This image: From IMSI's MasterClips/MasterPhotos 202,000 © 1997 Collection,
1895 Francisco Blvd. East, San Rafael, CA 94901-5506, USA
The Supernatural, the Paranormal and Eyewitness Testimony
Kirk Straughen
(Investigator 161, 2015 March)
Introduction
Eyewitness testimony often plays a central role as purported evidence
for the supernatural and the paranormal — from sightings of anomalous
animals and objects such as the Loch Ness monster, Bigfoot and flying
saucers to encounters with what are perceived as apparitions and
angels.
Although there is no doubt that, in most cases, the observer has seen
something the question is: how reliable is such evidence? Are people
good observers, how good are they at recalling events and are there any
factors that can interfere with these abilities?
The purpose of this article is to investigate the reliability of
eyewitness testimony, and the plan of the essay is as follows:
The first part examines the nature of memory and recall. The second
part investigates illusions and hallucinations that can lead to
erroneous conclusions about what has been perceived, and the third part
enquires as to why people interpret their experience in terms of the
supernatural and the paranormal.
Memories are not Videos
A common assumption many people have about memory is that it is similar
in some way to a movie — that memories are fixed entities stored in the
brain and can be recalled with the fidelity of a replayed DVD. This,
however, is an erroneous assumption:
It is a feature of human memory that we do not store information
exactly as it is presented to us. Rather, people extract from
information the gist, or underlying meaning.
In other words, people store information in the way that makes the most
sense to them. We make sense of information by trying to fit it into
schemas, which are a way of organizing information.
Schemas are mental 'units' of knowledge that correspond to frequently
encountered people, objects or situations. They allow us to make sense
of what we encounter in order that we can predict what is going to
happen and what we should do in any given situation. These schemas may,
in part, be determined by social values and therefore prejudice.
Schemas are therefore capable of distorting unfamiliar or unconsciously
'unacceptable' information in order to 'fit in' with our existing
knowledge or schemas. This can, therefore, result in unreliable
eyewitness testimony. (1)
When people try and make sense of events, particularly anomalous
phenomena, they often attempt to obtain congruence between what they
remember of an event with what they think they know and comprehend
about reality. As a consequence people's memories often undergo
alteration so that the event integrates with their conceptual schema of
the world.
The act of remembering is a reconstruction of an event rather than the
retrieval of indelible information. This is evidenced by the fact that
the memory of an eyewitness can be altered by the act of questioning,
where aspects of a recalled event can be unconsciously combined with
information provided by the questioner:
Many
researchers have created false memories in normal individuals; what is
more, many of these subjects are certain that the memories are real. In
one well-known study, Loftus and her colleague Jacqueline Pickrell gave
subjects written accounts of four events, three of which they had
actually experienced. The fourth story was fiction; it centered on the
subject being lost in a mall or another public place when he or she was
between four and six years old.
A relative provided realistic details for the false
story, such as a description of the mall at which the subject's parents
shopped. After reading each story, subjects were asked to write down
what else they remembered about the incident or to indicate that they
did not remember it at all. Remarkably about one third of the subjects
reported partially or fully remembering the false event. In two
follow-up interviews, 25 percent still claimed that they remembered the
untrue story, a figure consistent with the findings of similar studies.
(2)
Even without another party introducing misinformation, a person's
memory of events can be altered by the mere retelling of the event:
Those who have
witnessed a crime would do best not to tell anyone about it. Contrary
to what one might believe, a person's memory of an event is not
improved by retelling the story. Instead, the risk of an incorrect
account increases the more the story is retold and discussed.
"The most accurate witness statements come from people
who have seen a crime and then write down what happened before they
recount it or discuss it with anyone," says Farhan Sarwar.
However, it is quite unusual for witnesses to do this.
On the contrary, many want to immediately discuss what they have seen.
(3)
In addition to this 'Chinese whispers' effect, people rarely relate an
incident in a neutral way. Because people often tailor the story to an
audience, any bias on the part of the teller can distort the formation
of memory and recall:
Experiments conducted
by Barbara Tversky and Elizabeth Marsh corroborate the vulnerability of
human memory to bias. In one group of studies, participants were given
the "Roommate Story," a description of incidents involving his or her
two fictitious roommates. The incidents were categorized as annoying,
neutral, or socially "cool." Later, participants were asked to
neutrally recount the incidents with one roommate, to write a letter of
recommendation for one roommate's application to a fraternity or
sorority, or to write a letter to the office of student housing
requesting the removal of one of the roommates.
When later asked to recount the original story,
participants who had written biased letters recalled more of the
annoying or "cool" incidents associated with their letters. They also
included more elaborations consistent with their bias. These
participants made judgements based upon the annoying or social events
they discussed in their letters. Neutral participants made few
elaborations, and they also made fewer errors in their retelling, such
as attributing events to the wrong roommate. The study also showed that
participants writing biased letters recalled more biased information
for the character they wrote about, whereas the other roommate was
viewed neutrally. (4)
Although it is obvious that verbal misinformation can lead to the
creation of distorted memories, what is not so obvious, but has been
proved by experiments, is that body language can influence false memory
formation:
Dr Daniel Gurney from
the University of Hertfordshire interviewed 90 people about the
contents of a video they had watched. During the interviews,
researchers deliberately performed misleading hand gestures to suggest
inaccurate information about the detail in the video. These hand
gestures included chin stroking to suggest someone had a beard,
although the man in the video did not have a beard.
Dr Gurney and his team found that the interviewees were
three times more likely to recall seeing a beard when one was gestured
to them, than those interviewees who were not gestured to.
Other hand gestures used in the research included
touching a ring finger (to suggest a ring), grasping a wrist (to
suggest a watch) and pretending to pull on gloves. All of these
gestures implied details that did not actually appear in the video, and
the results were similar to those with the misinformation about the
beard.
Dr Gurney said: "A lot of research has shown that
eyewitnesses can be influenced by misleading questions, but this
research shows that gestures can also mislead, and sometimes without
eyewitnesses even realizing it. For those professionals in the police,
legal and other sensitive areas of work where questioning and recall of
detail is important, we need to make sure the significance of hand
gestures is fully taken on board." (5)
Finally, that there is a significant problem with eyewitness testimony
is borne out by the number of false convictions based on this evidence:
Eyewitness
misidentification is the single greatest cause of wrongful convictions
nationwide, playing a role in nearly 75% of convictions overturned
through DNA testing.
While eyewitness testimony can be persuasive evidence
before a judge or jury, 30 years of strong social science research has
proven that eyewitness identification is often unreliable. Research
shows that the human mind is not like a tape recorder; we neither
record events exactly as we see them, nor recall them like a tape that
has been rewound. Instead, witness memory is like any other evidence at
a crime scene; it must be preserved carefully and retrieved
methodically, or it can be contaminated. (6)
Illusions and Hallucinations
The previous section of this article explored the problems associated
with recall and false memory. In addition to these pitfalls there is
also the issue of illusion, which is a faulty observation arising from
a misperception of reality, such as mistaking a vague sound for a human
voice:
The psychological
phenomenon that causes some people to see or hear a vague or random
image or sound as something significant is known as pareidolia ...
There are a number of theories as to the cause of this
phenomenon. Experts say pareidolia provides a psychological
determination for many delusions that involve the senses. They believe
pareidolia could be behind numerous sightings of UFOs, Elvis and the
Loch Ness Monster and the hearing of disturbing messages on records
when they are played backwards.
Pareidolia often has religious overtones. A study in
Finland found that people who are religious or believe strongly in the
supernatural are more likely to see faces in lifeless objects and
landscapes. (7)
Why do we experience illusions? It is important to remember that
although our senses convey information to the brain, it is the brain
that constructs our experience of reality and that errors can creep in
during this process:
"The
brain is always constructing things, which is helping you survive. Some
of these constructions can be fiction," said Mark Changizi, a
neurobiologist and assistant professor of cognitive science at
Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute in Troy, N.Y.
Changizi came up with a theory to help explain why we
see illusions. He argued that illusions are due to the brain's attempt
to "see" the future. They occur during the slight time lag after light
reaches the retina in your eye, but before your brain translates it
into a visual perception.
According to Changizi, author of "The Vision
Revolution," when the brain attempts to generate a perception, it
basically is taking a guess at the near future by trying to
fast-forward a tenth of a second. As a result of this "neural delay,"
you might not be perceiving an image as it actually is, but as you
expect it might soon be.
"Illusions occur when the brain attempts to perceive
the future, and those perceptions don't match reality," he said.
Although there is no single reason illusions take place, Martinez-Conde
offered another possible explanation.
The brain is a limited structure with limited
resources, including its number of neurons, wires, and neuronal
connections, she suggested. "So in some cases, illusions may be due to
the brain's need to take shortcuts." Simply put, the brain might need
to quickly give more importance to some features in a visual scene than
others. (8)
In the case of illusions the observer misinterprets an external
stimulus. In the case of hallucinations, however, there is no external
stimulus, and although hallucinations are often associated with mental
illness healthy people can experience them as well:
Hallucinations are
perceptions in the absence of an external stimulus and are accompanied
by a compelling sense of their reality. They are a diagnostic feature
of schizophrenia, occurring in an estimated 60%-70% of people with this
disorder, with auditory hallucinations being the most common. However,
hallucinations are not only associated with illness but can also occur
in healthy individuals. For example, data from 6 community survey
studies in various countries indicate that 7%-30% of children and
adolescents report experiencing hallucinations. In the context of grief
after the death of a spouse, one-third to one-half of bereaved spouses
report hallucinations of the deceased. Transcultural influences may
also affect the distinction between reality and imagination as well as
the normalcy of visualizing images and ideas. (9)
Anthropomorphism, Suggestibility and
Apophenia
People can misconstrue reality, but why would they misconstrue it in a
way that leads them to the conclusion they have encountered paranormal
phenomena? Part of the reason is that paranormal beliefs are prevalent
in all societies and ages, arising as they do from our disposition to
see agents and purpose operating in nature as a result of
anthropomorphic thinking:
Ordinary social
cognitions seem to play a significant role in anthropomorphic thinking.
For example, by the age of five, children from every culture develop
the understanding that other humans have thoughts, feelings, and
beliefs that are different from their own. Humans may perceive the
mental states of nonhumans using this same cognitive process. In this
respect, anthropomorphism is a natural phenomenon that piggybacks on
other social cognitions.
Some anthropologists and evolutionary psychologists
have argued that humans have evolved to beware of a camouflaged
predator in the bush, to identify human forms and faces, or to perceive
the intentions of potential friends or foe. These self¬-protective
adaptations may cause us to be over-vigilant in our detection of
agency. Thus, in a better-safe-than-sorry strategy, humans often and
easily attribute intentionality or causality to invisible agents
(Boyer, 2001). After viewing a horror movie, we may be more inclined to
imagine a ghost or a burglar, rather than a tree branch tapping on the
window at night. (10)
In addition to anthropomorphic thinking, suggestion and the tendency to
find patterns and connections between events also appears to play a
part in the misinterpretation of mundane events as paranormal phenomena:
People are highly
social creatures, and this means they're also highly suggestible. If
you see another person behaving fearfully, for example, you're more
likely to feel fear, even if there's no obvious threat.
Suggestibility can fuel myths about ghosts and haunted
houses, particularly in an environment that seems creepy. If you stay
at an ostensibly haunted house, you're primed to see ghosts because
you've been told you might. This means you might interpret a strange
noise as a sign that a ghost is present, particularly if other people
seem frightened by the noise. Old and abandoned houses and locations
that have a scary story — such as a hotel where someone was killed, or
a home where someone committed suicide — can further prime your mind to
"see" ghosts, even when you might otherwise explain away unusual
apparitions and sounds ...
The human mind is incredibly adept at building
connections. This is the reason we're able to master complex math,
build seemingly intelligent machines, and remember vast quantities of
information. But this connection-building tendency can also cause
people to believe things that aren't true. Apophenia is the tendency to
see connections between unconnected events. This tendency can cause you
to interpret mundane experiences as supernatural. For example, if you
dream about your grandmother and then hear her favourite song on the
radio, you might conclude that she's communicating with you.
Pareidolia, a related phenomenon, occurs when people
"complete" incomplete images. There are mundane examples of this in
everyday life. Anyone who has noted that the front of a car looks like
a face is engaging in pareidolia. Pareidolia, however, can also cause
people to see ghostly images. Your mind, for example, might perceive a
cloud of dust as a face. Combined with high suggestibility, apophenia
and pareidolia can cause you to see things that aren't there. And while
the two behaviours can be associated with some mental health
conditions, they're normal cognitive processing mechanisms that
everyone engages in from time to time. (11)
Conclusion
Although eyewitness testimony is not useless it is not completely
reliable either. Many factors can contribute to the distortion of
memory, from the forgetting of vital facts to the inclusion of false
data.
Bias, the emotional state of the witness, the number of times the event
was recounted and the role the questioners played are just some of the
factors that can influence recall and must be taken into account when
investigating mundane events such as crimes. The need for at least
equal rigor, if not more, is obvious when investigating extraordinary
claims where preconceptions, the desire to believe and the tendency of
humans to see things that aren't there can interfere with rational
thought and dispassionate objectivity.
Bibliography:
(1) Eyewitness Testimony:
www.simplypsychology.org/eyewitness-testimony.html
(2) Why Science Tells Us Not to Rely on Eyewitness Accounts:
www.scientificamerican.com/article/do-the-eyes-have-it/
(3) Eyewitnesses are not as reliable as one might believe:
www.sciencedaily.com/releases/201l/01/110125092233.htm
(4) The Problem with Eyewitness Testimony:
http://agora.stanford.edu/sjls/Issue%20One/fisher&tversky.htm
(5) Influencing others through gestures: Pitfalls for eyewitnesses:
www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/04/120420105537.htm
(6) Eyewitness Misidentification:
www.innocenceproject.org/understandlEyewitness-Misidentification.php
(7) Pareidolia: Seeing Faces in Unusual Places:
www.livescience.com/25448-pareidolia.html
(8) Optical Illusions: When Your Brain Can't Believe Your Eyes:
http://abcnews.go.com/Health/EyeHealth/optical-illusions-eye-brain-agree/story?id=8455573&page=2
(9) On the neurobiology of hallucinations:
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2702442/
(10) The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology, Volume 1:
http://books.google.com.au/books?id=be0VYic5iWwC&pg=PA118&lpg=PAl18&dq=psychology+of+anthropomorphism&source=bl&ots=4HBUTgVIaW&sig=3j60mZ0loLmeWzwwnHsm8nvDVoM&hl=en&sa=X&ei=kA7GU8ylBoTm8AXSI4HYDg&ved=0CFYQ6AEwCTgU#V=onepage&q=psychology%20of%20anthropomorphism&f=false
(11) Psychological Explanations for Seemingly Paranormal Phenomena:
www.goodtherapy.org/blog/psychological-explanations-for-seemingly-paranormal-phenomena-0704137